next up previous contents
Next: 5.5 Time course of Up: 5 Aftereffect Experiments Previous: 5.3 Changes in the

Subsections

5.4 Changes in the response to test lines

  The weight changes described in the previous section result in changes in the pattern of the response to a test line. The differences in the responses were summarized as a single number (the perceived orientation) for each point on the curve in figure 5.3. This section will examine exactly how the activity patterns themselves change. First, it will look at how the cortical response to the adaptation stimulus changes, even though the perceived orientation does not change. It will then show the change in the response to test lines at 10° (where the peak direct effect was found) and 60° (where the peak indirect effect was found). The activity patterns are each plotted onto the orientation map from figure 4.3 so that the orientation preference of the activated neurons will be evident. These plots constitute predictions of what should be found in the cortex if the TAE is occurring as it does in the RF-LISSOM model.

5.4.1 Sharpening of the response to the adaptation line

Figure 5.6 shows the response to the adaptation line before and after adaptation.

 
Figure 5.6: Cortical response to the adaptation figure.
  The initial (pre-settling) and settled activity are shown for the adaptation Gaussian (a). The initial activity (due to the afferent weights only) is wide and diffuse, like the input pattern. It does not change with adaptation, since the afferent weights were fixed in this experiment. The settled activity did change, becoming more concentrated near the center where the input was the strongest. Still, the average orientation remained relatively constant, with equal areas of colors on either side of 0° activated both before and after adaptation.
\begin{figure}
\begin{minipage}
{\textwidth}
 \centering
 \begin{minipage}
{\two...
 ...\baselineskip}\newline
\usebox {\orkey}

 \end{minipage} \newline

 \end{figure}

The perceived orientation was approximately 0° both before and after adaptation (figure 5.3). However, there were significant changes in the settled activity (i.e., the activity remaining after lateral interactions have been allowed to settle). More neurons near the center of the cortex, where the input was strongest, were activated after adaptation. Fewer were activated in the outlying areas. Thus adaptation has sharpened the activity pattern.

The sharpening is caused by the strengthening of inhibition between activated neurons -- only the areas with the strongest activation remain active after settling. The rest of the neurons initially activated in figure 5.6b are silenced by the active ones in the center. The sharpening effect is present even before adaptation, which is why the settled activity differs from the initial activity, but it gets stronger with adaptation. The inhibitory weight changes and the activity changes can be related to each other by comparing the areas showing changes between figure 5.6c and 5.6d with the areas showing increased inhibition in figure 5.5a. The increased inhibition has effectively created a group of competitive neurons where only a central region of strongly activated neurons is allowed to continue responding.

None of these changes show up in the TAE curve in figure 5.3 at 0° because the distribution of activated units contains approximately the same proportion of each orientation preference both before and after adaptation. Compared to figure 5.6c, there are more green units (orientation preference less than 0°) activated in the center of figure 5.6d. At the same time, there are more blue units (orientation preference greater than 0°) activated just outside that area. These changes cancel out for the adaptation orientation, since they are centered upon it. As discussed in the next section, the direct TAE occurs for test lines of slightly different orientations, where the changes do not cancel out.

5.4.2 Expansion of small angles (direct effect)

 

For the peak of the direct effect, the perceived orientation of a 10° test line shifted away from 0° by several degrees (figure 5.3). This change is quite subtle but evident in the activity patterns for the +10° test line in figure 5.7.

 
Figure 5.7: Cortical response at the peak direct effect.
  For the input pattern shown (10° counterclockwise from the adaptation inputs), the settled activity changes significantly with adaptation. Fewer of the units encoding orientations greater than +10° (i.e., the cyan and blue areas) are activated. Slightly more of the regions encoding orientations less than +10° (the green and yellow-green areas) are activated. The net effect is an angle expansion: the average orientation changes from +14.5° to +16.9°. The change is perceived as the direct TAE.
\begin{figure}
\begin{minipage}
{\textwidth}
 \centering
 \begin{minipage}
{\two...
 ...\baselineskip}\newline
\usebox {\orkey}

 \end{minipage} \newline

 \end{figure}

After adaptation, fewer of the units encoding orientations less than +10° are activated (the cyan and blue areas). Slightly more of the units encoding orientations greater than +10° are activated (the yellow, yellow-green, and green areas). The net effect is a shift of the perceived orientation away from 0°, resulting in the direct TAE.

The changes are once again due to the strengthening of the inhibitory connections. As shown in figure 5.5a, the inhibitory connections between the green, cyan, and blue neurons were strengthened. This test line initially activated the green orientations most strongly, so lateral interactions allowed those neurons to silence the weakly-activated cyan and blue areas. The increased inhibition between neurons active during adaptation results in competition between them for activation -- only a subset will remain active at a given time. Because these changes occurred only for neurons preferring orientations closer to 0° than 10°, the perceived orientation shifts away from 0°. Thus direct tilt aftereffects are occurring in the RF-LISSOM model exactly as predicted by the lateral inhibition theory.

5.4.3 Contraction of large angles (indirect effect)

  For the peak of the indirect effect, the perceived orientation of a 60° test line shifted towards 0° by nearly a degree (figure 5.3). This change is again subtle, yet still evident in the activity patterns for the +60° test line in figure 5.8.
 
Figure 5.8: Cortical response at the peak indirect effect.
  For the input pattern shown (60° counterclockwise of the adaptation pattern), the settled activity again changes significantly with adaptation, but in the opposite direction from that of figure 5.7. More of the units encoding orientations greater +60° (i.e., the green areas) are activated, since they are no longer inhibited as strongly as before adaptation. The regions encoding orientations less than the testing orientation (the yellow and red areas) show little change. The net effect is an angle attraction: the average orientation changes from +58.3° to +57.4°. The change is perceived as the indirect TAE.
\begin{figure}
\begin{minipage}
{\textwidth}
 \centering
 \begin{minipage}
{\two...
 ...\baselineskip}\newline
\usebox {\orkey}

 \end{minipage} \newline

 \end{figure}

After adaptation, more of the units encoding orientations smaller than +60° are activated (the green areas). The same units encoding orientations greater than +60° are activated both before and after adaptation; these units show no change because they were never activated during adaptation. The net effect is a shift of the perceived orientation towards 0°, resulting in the indirect TAE.

The indirect effect, true to its name, results only indirectly from the strengthening of the inhibitory connections. The inhibition between the green, cyan, and blue neurons was strengthened during adaptation. Of those, only the green neurons were activated by this test line, since it is quite distant from the adaptation line. Thus the stronger inhibitory connections themselves did not change the response pattern significantly, since the targets were already inactive.

However, since the total inhibitory connection strength to each neuron is limited (equation 3.4), the increases in strength among the green, cyan, and blue neurons come at the expense of all the other inhibitory connections to the green neurons. As shown in figure 5.5b, the weakened connections are spread out over neurons with a wide range of orientation preferences. The weakened connections include those to nearly all of the units activated for this test line. Thus those units inhibit the green areas much less than they did before adaptation. Consequently, more of the green neurons remain active throughout the settling process. Before adaptation, many of them had been silenced by the other neurons closer to the orientation of the test line. The net effect of these changes is that the perceived orientation shifts towards 0°.

The magnitude of the indirect effect is smaller than the direct effect because fewer adapted neurons are involved. Most of the neurons showed little change; only those few closest to the adaptation orientation were affected. For the direct effect, many neurons were affected -- a number of those preferring orientations greater than that of the test line became less active, and some of the ones preferring smaller orientations became more active. These changes add up to a greater difference in the perceived orientation for the direct effect than for the indirect effect.

The indirect effect in RF-LISSOM is a simple consequence of the normalization of inhibitory weights after they have been adjusted by Hebbian learning. This explanation for the indirect effect was not discovered until the TAE was modeled here, thus underscoring the importance of building computational models. Chapter 6 will discuss the biological evidence for this theory of the indirect effect, and will compare it to the evidence for other theories that have been proposed.

The model has allowed us to look into the cortex as it is processing and see the changes as they occur. No other computational model has allowed the tilt aftereffect to be studied so concretely. As also discussed in chapter 6, it may be possible to obtain similar plots for the cortex to compare with these results.


next up previous contents
Next: 5.5 Time course of Up: 5 Aftereffect Experiments Previous: 5.3 Changes in the
James A. Bednar
9/19/1997